“What
lies behind us and what lies before us are tiny matters compared to
what lies within us,” Ralph Waldo Emerson (“Ralph Waldo Emerson
Quotes”).
Actions
are the direct result of our psyches. And what our psyche generates
—
subconscious wants, needs, and intentions; conscious wants, needs, and
intentions
—
determines our actions (Priddy). On the outside stands a
single body over his victim, yet on the inside is a man’s spectrum of
conscious and subconscious emotions and intentions which provoked him to
commit the crime.
Something made the man murder a fellow man
—
but what? Why is that something there? How can we stop that something from causing another man to murder? This is where the humble forensic psychologist comes in.
A behavior analysist’s job is to fully understand this spectrum of intentions and emotions
—
this something that makes you you, and me me.
Training, experiencing, and specializing in man’s behavior, they are
the true experts in criminal psyches...so why does the court belittle
them and their career? To this day, forensic psychology and behavior analysis reports are not
admissible as direct evidence
—
more commonly known as hard evidence
—
nor
are the psychologists themselves allowed to testify without a judge’s
opinionated consent in a court of law (Ramsland 223). However, forensic
psychology is a factual and trustworthy profession, and this regulation
undervalues it greatly, disregarding the concrete foundations of
behavior analysis. Forensic psychology reports should be admissible as direct evidence in court.
Why is this such a controversial notion? I’m convinced the people as a
whole don’t fully understand what it is they do; therefore, this
profession should be brought to light.
Skeptics
scoff at the thought of a profile--the forensic psychology
report--being suitable in any court of law, saying forensic psychology
is “voodoo,” and the analysts are merely “gratified mystics,”(Grezlak)
but they are far from the truth. Forensic psychology reports are
legitimate sources of evidence which deserve to be treated as just that.
The
FBI has a unit for all forensic psychologists: the Behavioral Analysis
Unit, or BAU. Those agents’ job is to use what they know of the
suspects’
—
also referred to as “unsubs”, meaning the unknown subjects
(Ramsland 287)
—
behavior and predict their future behavior
retrospectively and prospectively. Retrospective profiling uses details
at crime scenes to assess personality traits and predict future
behavior, while prospective profiling relies on past unsubs’
characteristics and behaviors to compare them to present unsubs’
(Ramsland 150).
The
foundation of forensic psychology revolves around the concept that
everything we do, as humans, is intentional
—
consciously and
subconsciously. Subconscious choices stem from memories; for example, I
hit you with a stick and it hurts. I swing the stick again, and you duck
out of the way subconsciously. Your brain is intentionally choosing an
action based on its past experiences (Priddy). Likewise, in many cases
when a mother abuses her son at a young age, and that child grows up to
have a violent outrage, he will most likely target women in their
mid-40’s because, though he may not realise it, subconsciously that
unsub is seeking revenge on his mother for all the torment and abuse
(Munro).
“‘I
use a formula, 'How plus Why equals Who,’’” former BAU chief John
Douglas says. “‘If we can answer the hows and whys in a crime, we
generally can come up with the solution,’” (Ramsland). This is the
foundation of forensic psychology. Through crime scene forensics,
victimology, and apprehension, the overall goal of behavior analysis is
to find means and motive
—
the how and why.
The
process of victimology is the main source of retrospective profiling
(Ramsland 34), as it is both credible and reliable. Who the unsub
targets, where the victim is captured, how the unsub handles the victim,
how he/she chooses to murder, and where the disposal site of the victim
is tells forensic psychologists nearly everything they need to know to
put together an accurate profile of the unsub. The BAU agents decipher
what type of transportation the unsub has, what social class the unsub
falls under, what type of workspace the unsub must hold the victim(s)
in, whether or not the unsub knew the victim(s), etc. by victimology
(Ramsland 34). All of these questions answered reflect many personal
characteristics of the unsub, as they narrow down the suspect pool
sufficiently. Additionally, if the unsub is a serial offender, by
identifying the remains, the agents working the case can predict the
type of torment the next victim will likely suffer.
Victimology
also includes basic apprehension techniques, cross-checking the victims
contacts and whereabouts to determine what trait they all have in
common (Ramsland 34). This often overlaps with graphical profiling
—
a
profiling technique predicting the transportation limits of an unsub as
well as where he/she may live or have their workspace (Young 5).
Theresa
M. Young, honors student at Northeastern University, battles the
validity of many profiling techniques: “Upon examination, these
typologies [geographic profiling, investigative psychology, criminal
investigative analysis, and behavioral evidence analysis] are flawed, as
are their practical application processes, and have implications in
training, accessibility, and utility,” (Young iii). To a certain extent,
Young is accurate by saying that some components of certain profiling
techniques are faulty.
There
is no set template for a violent offender’s behavior patterns;
therefore, prospective profiling can be inaccurate. Racial profiling,
using statistics of which races have committed which crimes in the past,
is argued to be extremely racist. Geographical profiling, an
“investigative methodology” that uses the “locations of a connected
series of crimes” to determine the most logical region of an offender’s
workspace or home (Harries), is said to be inaccurate because it
revolves around the assumption that a killer’s “dump site” is directly
related to where the unsub lives (Young 7).
With
an ultimate goal of limiting the suspect pool, traits such as age and
sex are most beneficial. A profile constructed through behavioral
analysis, however, does not include the age and sex of the unsub. BAU
agents regularly refer to these components as “problem characteristics”
because they can rarely be determined from the crime scene evidence and
victimology (Turvey 342), and must instead be inferred through
prospective profiling
—
the type of profiling least accepted by the
public because of it’s roots in statistics (Ramsland). They rely on
ideas of what behavior is most likely from certain groups
—
stereotypes.
For example, women are generally less aggressive than men, and 80 year
olds tend not to strangle teenagers. This prospective profiling, though
still based on neuroscience studies, gets poor feedback because its
paramount source of information is statistics; unquestionably, some
offenders won’t match up exactly with any prior statistic, and therefore
prospective profiling will be fallacious.
At
this point in time, behavioral analysis isn’t admissible in court as
hard evidence; rather, it’s the judge’s decision whether or not the
forensic psychologist is allowed to even testify. “Because a profile is
based on behavioral science, which uses probability assessments, whether
a profiler can testify is often subject to the judge’s discretion...if a
profiler is not admissible in court...profilers [are permitted to]
offer assistance to court personnel in recognizing personality
traits...[and] clarify other aspects of forensic evidence, explain
technical angles, and help reconstruct the crime. They can offer
information...to shed light on how offenders might have been thinking
when they made certain decisions,”(Ramsland 223-224). Though this
regulation has a technical loophole, it is still unfounded, biased, and
defective.
The
law says that a forensic psychologist can investigate as much as they
want, yet cannot speak in the court unless the judge deems them worthy
(Ramsland 223). So essentially, if that judge is a skeptic, there’s no
way that analyst--no matter how much experience or education he/she has
gained
—
would be allowed to testify. Mentioned above, the loophole to
this is for an analyst to help prosecutors with strategy to get the
unsub convicted.
A
real-life example of this unjust law is the trial of Christopher
Distefano, a murderer. In this trial, Dr. Richard Walter, a Michigan
prison psychologist, wasn’t allowed to testify by order of the judge,
and neither could Robert Hazelwood, “who worked for 16 years in the
Behavioral Science Unit.” The judge didn’t see that these two highly
qualified analysts were suitable to testify because the reports had“
little probative value,"(Grezlak), which meant that they had little
proof or evidence.
Direct
evidence, more commonly known as hard evidence, is practically the only
way to convict an unsub. It is evidence which requires no thinking,
consideration, or proof of its existence, proving the issue as a fact
without inference or presumption (“Real and Demonstrative Evidence”).
One wouldn’t question the existence of a fingerprint or blood spatter,
so why would anyone question the existence of the neuroscience behind
behavioral analysis and forensic psychology?
Neuroscience,
the study of the brain and its functions, is the stem of all
behavior-knowing, and has had many advances throughout this past
decade
—
advantages such as the functions of the amygdala, an
almond-shaped set of neurons deep in the temporal lobe, is linked to the
psyche’s responses in fear and pleasure (Bruzza, Hurlemann, Welsh,
Hughes). People with damage to their amygdalae are more likely to take
bigger risks for smaller gains (DeMartino), such risks include robbery
and homicide. Also with damage to the amygdala, much emotion is lost:
decisions don’t take into account any emotional processing (Stenson).
Damage
to the amygdala can also mean the development of personality disorders
such as narcissism, psychosis, sociopathy, and psychopathy
—
each of
which are diagnosed in hundreds or killers time and time again (Ramsley
87). Knowing this helps considerably with narrowing down a suspect pool.
For example, if an unsub has kidnapped Suzie, and the BAU has narrowed
down the suspect pool to George O’Malley, a philosopher diagnosed with
ADHD, or Jimmy McClean, a mechanic diagnosed with sociopathy, the house
agents will search first is Jimmy McClean’s, primarily because
statistics show that a large number of violent offenders are diagnosed
with disorders such as this one.
With
each study performed, neuroscience progresses and new neurological
information is discovered, further developing the accuracy of
prospective profiling and retrospective profiling alike. Each statistic,
each test, each biopsy is a lead to getting our streets cleared of
violent offenders, which is why forensic psychology needs to be taken
seriously in a court of law. Reports shouldn’t have to go through a
judge’s opinion before being offered to the jury. Doesn’t anyone see how
biased and unjust that is? Behavioral analysis gets results; that is an
undeniable fact. It’s truly a science and should be treated as such in
every courtroom across the globe.
Robert
Brault once wrote, “One can only imagine how effective justice might be
if admissible in a court of law;” (“Justice Quotes”) he could not be
more accurate. With regulations like these existing in the government,
no wonder so many criminals remain free. And what happens when criminals
are allowed to walk away without a scratch? Vigilantes, or men and
women of society who take it upon themselves to seek out and murder
criminals who were rendered not guilty--a.k.a., more crime. If the court
just audits its rulings to be just and righteous, hundreds of killers
will be locked up for good, and our streets will be safer. However, as
Brault wrote, it’s unlikely the court will ever be just and true...we
can only hope.
No comments:
Post a Comment